Journal of Global Health: Home Journal of Global Health
Watch: Survival: The Story of Global Health - FREE
JoGH Recommends:

Peer Review Conference



Creative Commons Licence
This work is licensed under a
Creative Commons Attribution
4.0 International License
.


Xiaohui Yu, Liwen Zheng, Wenjie Jiang, and Dongfeng Zhang

Abstract

Background

We conducted a meta-analysis to explore the relationship between exposure to air pollution and the risk of cognitive impairment of longitudinal cohort studies.

Methods

PubMed, Web of Science and Wan Fang databases were searched for relevant articles of longitudinal cohort studies published between January 1950 and September 2019. The pooled relative ratio (RR) and 95% confidence interval (CI) were calculated using the random effect model.

Results

Ten articles involving 519 247 cases among 12 523 553 participants were included in this meta-analysis. The pooled RR of cognitive impairment per 5 μg/m3 increments in exposure to particulate matter ≤2.5 μm (PM2.5) was 1.08 (95% CI = 1.03, 1.13; I2 = 82.2%; Pheterogeneity<0.001). No association was found between nitrogen dioxide/nitrogen oxide (NO2/NOx) and ozone (O3) and cognitive impairment. For PM2.5 exposure, in subgroup analysis, the above-mentioned significant positive association was found among studies conducted in population (RRper 5 μg/m3 = 1.05; 95% CI = 1.01,1.09; I2 = 57.4%; Pheterogeneity = 0.016), in North America (RRper 5 μg/m3 = 1.13; 95% CI = 1.01,1.26; I2 = 86.7%; Pheterogeneity<0.001) and with follow-up duration >10 years (RRper 5 μg/m3 = 1.10; 95% CI = 1.03,1.17; I2 = 86.3%; Pheterogeneity<0.001).

Conclusions

This meta-analysis suggests that exposure to PM2.5 might increase the risk of cognitive impairment.


Air pollution, mainly particulate matter (PM) and gas pollutants [1], is one of the ten threats to global health in 2019, causing 7 million people dying prematurely every year [2]. Updated estimations from the World Health Organization (WHO) show that around 90% of the worldwide people are breathing polluted air [3]. Hence, air pollution has become the greatest environmental hazard to public health.

Cognitive impairment, mainly encompassing both Alzheimer disease (AD) and dementia [4-6], is the 5th leading cause of global death in 2016 [7], occurring nearly 10 million new cases every year [8]. The cause of cognitive impairment is considered to be a combination of genetic and environmental factors. As a modifiable environmental factor, air pollution plays a crucial role in central nervous system diseases, including cognitive impairment [9]. Accumulating animal studies indicated that air pollutants could lead to neuroinflammation and oxidative stress [9-11], which were involved in pathological evidence of cognitive impairment [12-16]. Based on these findings, many studies have evaluated the association between air pollution and cognitive impairment. Specifically, for PM2.5 exposure, a positive association was found in four studies [17-19], whereas no significant association was shown in other studies [20-25]. For NO2/NOx exposure, although the risk of cognitive impairment was illustrated in two studies [17,18], the link was not manifested in other studies [20,21,25,26]. For O3 exposure, one study [17] revealed an inverse relationship with cognitive impairment, while another study [20] found a positive relationship with cognitive impairment. In light of the inconsistencies among the above epidemiological studies, we conducted a meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies to synthesize the results of existing studies to evaluate the relationship of air pollution with cognitive impairment.

METHODS

This meta-analysis was conducted according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines [27].

Search strategy

We searched all relevant articles in English or Chinese from PubMed, Web of Science and Wan Fang databases published between January 1950 and September 2019. The following search strategy was used: (air pollution or particulate matter or carbon monoxide or nitrogen dioxide or nitrogen oxide or sulfur dioxide or ozone) And (cognitive impairment or dementia). The exhaustive search process in PubMed is shown in Table S1 in the Online Supplementary Document. Furthermore, we reviewed the reference lists of retrieved articles to identify additional relevant articles.

Inclusion criteria

The included studies ought to meet the following criteria: (1) longitudinal cohort studies published in English or Chinese; (2) the exposure of interest was particulate (PM2.5, particles with an aerodynamic diameter ≤2.5 μm; PM10, particles with an aerodynamic diameter ≤10 μm or PM2.5-10, particles with an aerodynamic diameter 2.5-10) or gaseous (NO2, nitrogen dioxide; NOx, nitrogen oxide; O3, Ozone; CO, carbon monoxide; SO2, sulfur dioxide) air pollutants; (3) the outcome of interest was cognitive impairment mainly including dementia and AD; (4) Multivariate adjusted odds ratio (OR), relative risk (RR) or hazard ratio (HR) and their 95% confidence interval (CI) of cognitive impairment with exposures to air pollution with per unit increase in pollutant concentrations (μg/m3, mg/m3, ppb, or ppm) were available, or sufficient data could be used to convert these results.

Exclusion criteria

(1) Review articles and studies were written in other languages instead of English or Chinese; (2) studies that did not estimate the relationship between air pollution and the risk of cognitive impairment; (3) results cannot be converted into RR and 95% CI with per unit increase in particulate (PM2.5, PM10, or PM2.5-10) or gaseous (NO2, NOx, O3, CO, SO2) air pollutants concentrations (μg/m3, mg/m3, ppb, or ppm); (4) duplicated articles; (5) not cohort studies.

Two investigators independently performed the literature search. If there was disagreement on an article, the consensus was reached through discussion. If data appeared in more than one study, we would select the most comprehensive data.

Data extraction

Data extracted from each identified study by two investigators independently as follows: the first author’s name, publication year, study areas and country, age range or mean age of participants, sex, follow-up duration, sample size, number of cases, measurement of exposure, the source of outcome, RRs (we presented all results with RR for simplicity) and their 95% CI with per unit increase in particulate (PM2.5, PM10, or PM2.5-10) or gaseous (NO2, NOx, O3, CO, SO2) air pollutants concentrations (μg/m3, mg/m3, ppb, or ppm), adjusted covariates.

The included cohort literature was assessed by the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) [28].

Statistical analysis

We use the inverse variance and the number of participants to weight the study-specific log RRs that can calculate the pooled RR with corresponding 95% CI to evaluate the relationship between air pollution and cognitive impairment. For consistency, all RRs were standardized to an increment of 5 μg/m3 of particular matter (PM2.5, PM2.5-10, and PM10) concentration and 5 ppb of gaseous (NO2, NOx, O3, CO, SO2) concentration. It is hypothesized that there is a linear relationship of exposure to air pollution and cognitive impairment. If studies reported RRu per u units not in per 5 μg/m3 of particular matter (PM2.5, PM2.5-10, and PM10) or 5 ppb of gaseous (NO2, NOx, O3, CO, SO2), the RRstandardized was computed by the following formula [29]:

RRstandardized = RRuincrement unit (eg, 5)/u

Where u represents the increment utilized in the original study to assess the effects. We used a unit conversion factor: 1 ppb = 1.88 μg/m3 for NO2/NOx and 1 ppb = 1.96 μg/m3 for O3, and standard condition is ambient pressure of 1 atmosphere and a temperature of 25°C [30].

We adopted to I2 statistic to evaluate the heterogeneity, and the I2 values of 0%, 25%, 50%, and 75% represent no, low, moderate and high heterogeneity, respectively [31]. The random effect model (REM) was utilized as the pooling method. Meta-regression with restricted maximum likelihood estimation was used to explore the potential covariates that may exert substantial impacts on between-study heterogeneity [32]. Subgroup analysis was conducted by the source of outcome, study areas, follow-up duration, and adjustment status for comorbidity and smoking. We carried out the leave-one-out sensitivity analysis to examine whether individual study influences between-study heterogeneity [33]. Influence analysis was performed excluding one study at a time to ascertain whether the aggregate results could be significantly affected by a single study [34]. Additionally, the funnel plot and Egger regression asymmetry test were used to estimate the publication bias [35]. All statistical analyses were done using STATA Version 15.0 (STATA Corporation, College Station, TX, USA). A two-sided P ≤ 0.05 was equated statistically significant.

RESULTS

Search results and study characteristics

On the basis of search strategy, a total of 11 090 articles were identified, involving 1085 articles from PubMed, 382 articles from Web of Science, 9622 articles from Wan Fang and one article from reference lists. After removing 207 articles because of duplicates, there are 10 883 articles left. Reviewing the title and abstract later, forty-one articles were remaining. We further excluded thirty-one articles after reviewing the full-text. The flow diagram of the literature search is displayed in Figure 1. The detailed reason for full-text reviewed articles exclusion is provided in Table S2 in the Online Supplementary Document.

Figure 1.  Flow diagram of the literature search for the study selection process.
jogh-10-010417-F1


Ultimately, ten articles [17-26], including 11 studies on PM2.5, 7 studies on NO2/NOx and 4 studies on O3, were satisfied the inclusion criteria and were enrolled in this meta-analysis. Eight articles [18,21-26] were population-based studies and two articles [19,20] were hospital-based studies. The duration of follow-up ranged from 4 to 18 years, with three articles [17,22,23] following equal to or less than 10 years and seven articles [18-21,24-26] more than 10 years. Concerning the study areas, four articles [17,20,24,26] were performed in Europe, five articles [18,19,21,23,25] in North America and one article [22] in Asia.

The quality assessment indicated that the Newcastle-Ottawa score of cohort studies was not less than 7. Specific quality assessments are presented in Table S3 in the Online Supplementary Document. The characteristics of the studies are shown in Table 1.

Table 1.  Characteristics of included studies on the association between air pollution and the risk of cognitive impairment*
Author, (year) [Ref.] Country Age (year), Sex Follow-up years Sample size (cases) Exposure Exposure measurement Outcome Source of outcome RR (95% CI) Adjustments
Carey (2018) [17] Britain 50-79, Both 9 130 978 (2181) PM2.5, NO2, O3 Dispersion mode Dementia Population based PM2.5, original value (Per IQR = 0.95μg/m3): 1.06 (1.02, 1.12); equivalent value (Per 5 μg/m3): 1.36 (1.11, 1.82). Age, sex, ethnicity, smoking, body mass index, index of multiple deprivation comorbidity (ischemic heart disease, stroke, heart failure).
PM2.5 (traffic), original value (Per IQR = 0.58μg/m3): 1.08 (1.01, 1.16); equivalent value (Per 5 μg/m3): 1.94 (1.09, 3.59).
NO2, original value (Per IQR = 7.47μg/m3): 1.16 (1.05, 1.27); equivalent value (Per 5 ppb): 1.21 (1.06, 1.35).
O3, original value (Per IQR = 5.56μg/m3): 0.85 (0.76, 0.94); equivalent value (Per 5 ppb): 0.75 (0.62, 0.90).
Oudin (2016) [26] Sweden 55-85, Both 18 1806 (302) NOx LUR Dementia Population based Original value (Per 10 μg/m3): 1.05 (0.98, 1.12); equivalent value (Per 5 ppb): 1.05 (0.98, 1.11). Baseline age, sex, education, physical activity, smoking, body mass index, waist–hip ratio, alcohol, ApoE4, baseline medical history of diabetes, stroke and hypertension.
Oudin (2018) [24] Sweden 55-85, Both 18 1806 (302) PM2.5 Dispersion mode Dementia Population based Wood burning, original value (Per 1μg/m3): 1.55 (1.00, 2.41); equivalent value (Per 5 μg/m3): 8.95 (1.00, 81.30). Age, sex, PM2.5 from traffic exhaust, PM2.5 from residential wood burning, physical activity, smoking, body mass index, alcohol and waist-hip-ratio.
Traffic exhaust, original value (Per 1μg/m3): 1.14 (0.59, 2.23); equivalent value (Per 5 μg/m3): 1.93 (0.07, 55.15).
Chen (2017) [18] Canada 55-85, Both 13 2 066 639 (257 816) PM2.5, NO2, O3 LUR Dementia Population based PM2.5, original value (Per IQR = 4.8μg/m3): 1.04 (1.03, 1.05); equivalent value (Per 5 μg/m3): 1.04 (1.03, 1.05). Age, sex, history of diabetes, hypertension, coronary heart disease, stroke, congestive heart failure, arrhythmia, traumatic brain injury, income quintile, urban residency, north/south indicator, census division-level unemployment rate, education and recent immigrants.
NO2, original value (Per IQR = 14.2 ppb): 1.10 (1.08, 1.12); equivalent value (Per 5 ppb): 1.034 (1.027,1.041).
O3, original value (Per IQR = 6.3 ppb): 0.98 (0.96, 1.00); equivalent value (Per 5 ppb): 0.98 (0.97, 1.00).
Cerza (2019) [20] Italy 65-100, Both 13 350 844 (21 548) PM2.5, NO2, NOx O3 LUR and chemical transport model Dementia Hospital based PM2.5, original value (Per 5 μg/m3): 0.99 (0.96, 1.02). Age, education, place of birth, marital status, area-based socioeconomic position with baseline hazard function stratified by sex.
NO2, original value (Per 10 μg/m3): 0.97 (0.96, 0.99); equivalent value (Per 5 ppb): 0.97 (0.96, 0.99).
NOx, original value (Per 20 μg/m3): 1.01 (1.00, 1.02); equivalent value (Per 5 ppb): 1.005 (1.000, 1.009).
O3, original value (Per 10 μg/m3):1.06 (1.03, 1.08); equivalent value (Per 5 ppb): 1.06 (1.03, 1.08).
Kioumourtzoglou (2016) [19] America 75, Both 12 9 817 806 (230 463) PM2.5 The US Environmental Protection Agency’s (EPA) Air Quality System (AQS) database Dementia Hospital based Original value (Per 5 μg/m3): 1.46 (1.29, 1.66). Age, sex, race, individual socioeconomic status and for any prior cardiopulmonary admission and severity of disease.
Ilango (2019) [21] Canada 60, Both 18 34 391 (2559) PM2.5, NO2 LUR Dementia Population based PM2.5, original value (Per 10 μg/m3): 1.29 (0.99, 1.64); equivalent value (Per 5 μg/m3): 1.14 (0.99, 1.28). Age, sex, education, marital status, income quintile, smoking status, body mass index, physical activity, rural residence and northern region; area level: recent immigrants, unemployment and education.
NO2, original value (Per 5 ppb): 1.10 (0.99, 1.19).
Yu (2016) [25] Canada ≥65, Both 14 5249 (1346) PM2.5, NO2 Satellite imaging and LUR Dementia Population based PM2.5, original value (Per 10 μg/m3): 1.14 (0.93, 1.40); equivalent value (Per 5 μg/m3): 1.07 (0.96, 1.18). Age, sex, vitamin use, non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drugs, exercise, hypertension, and obesity.
NO2, original value (Per 5 ppb): 0.98 (0.93, 1.03).
Jung (2015) [22] China ≥65, Both 10 95 690 (1399) PM2.5, O3 Monitoring stations Alzheimer disease Population based PM2.5, original value (Per 13.21μg/m3): 1.03 (0.95, 1.11); equivalent value (Per 5 μg/m3): 1.01 (0.98, 1.04). Age, sex, income, diabetes mellitus, hypertensive disease, myocardial infarction, stroke, asthma and chronic obstructive pulmonary disease.
O3, original value (Per 9.63 ppb): 1.06 (1.00, 1.12); equivalent value (Per 5 ppb): 1.03 (1.00,1.06).
Loop (2013) [23] America 64, Men: 45% 4 20 150 (1633) PM2.5 Ground-level and Earth-orbiting satellite measurements Cognitive impairment Population based Original value (Per 10 μg/m3): 0.98 (0.72, 1.34); equivalent value (Per 5 μg/m3): 0.99 (0.85, 1.16). Age, race, region, education, body mass index, income, PM2.5, length of follow up, the potential confounders temperature,season,incident stroke, smoking status, alcohol use, exercis and comorbidities (presence of depressive symptoms, diabetes,dyslipidemia, hypertension).

RR – relative risk, PM2.5 – particulate matter ≤2.5 μm, NO2 – nitrogen dioxide NOx – nitrogen oxide, O3 – ozone; LUR – land-use regression model; Equivalent value, RR standardized = RRuincrement unit (eg, 5)/u is the equation for converting the original value to the equivalent value. RR standardized denotes equivalent value, RRu denotes original value, u denotes the increment of PM2.5, NO2, NOx, O3 concentration used in the original study, ppb - parts per billion

*Conversion factors: NO2/NOx: 1 ppb = 1.88 μg/m3; O3, 1 ppb = 1.96 μg/m3.



Quantitative synthesis

Table 2 shows the summary risk estimates of cognitive impairment for air pollution according to study characteristics.

Table 2.  Summary risk estimates of cognitive impairment for air pollution according to study characteristics
Study characteristics PM2.5 RR (95% CI) I2 (%) Pheterogeneity NO2/NOx RR (95% CI) I2 (%) Pheterogeneity
N N
All studies 11 1.08 (1.03, 1.13) 82.2% <0.001 7 1.02 (0.99, 1.04) 93.4% <0.001
Source of outcome:
Population-based 9 1.05 (1.01, 1.09) 57.4% 0.016 5 1.048 (1.004, 1.094) 68.5% 0.013
Hospital-based 2 1.20 (0.82, 1.75) 97.1% <0.001 2 0.99 (0.95, 1.02) 94.7% <0.001
Study areas:
Europe 5 1.34 (0.94, 1.89) 73.4% 0.005 4 1.01 (0.98, 1.05) 90.1% <0.001
North America 5 1.13 (1.01, 1.26) 86.7% <0.001 3 1.03 (0.98, 1.08) 66.4% 0.051
Asia 1 1.01 (0.98, 1.04) / /
Follow-up duration:
≤10 4 1.11(0.95, 1.31) 70.4% 0.018 1 1.21 (1.06, 1.35) / /
>10 7 1.10 (1.03, 1.17) 86.3% <0.001 4 1.01 (0.99, 1.03) 94.4% <0.001
Adjusted comorbidity:
Yes 7 1.10 (1.03, 1.17) 85.3% <0.001 4 1.04 (0.99, 1.09) 72.6% 0.012
No 4 1.07 (0.90, 1.26) 64.1% 0.039 3 1.00 (0.97, 1.04) 91.2% <0.001
Adjusted smoking:
Yes 6 1.21 (1.00, 1.46) 57.0% 0.040 3 1.10 (1.02, 1.19) 53.2% 0.118
No 5 1.06 (1.01, 1.11) 90.2% <0.001 4 1.00 (0.98, 1.03) 96.5% <0.001


PM2.5 exposure and the risk of cognitive impairment

Eleven studies [17-25] comprising 519 247 cases among 12 523 553 participants were included. The pooled RR of cognitive impairment per 5 μg/m3 increments in exposure to PM2.5 was 1.08 (95% CI = 1.03, 1.13; I2 = 82.2%; Pheterogeneity<0.001; Figure 2). A positive significant association (RRper 5 μg/m3= 1.36; 95% CI = 1.24, 1.50; I2 = 99.7%; Pheterogeneity<0.001; Fig. S1 in the Online Supplementary Document) of cognitive impairment with PM2.5 exposure was found after sensitivity analysis with the number of participants as the weight.

Figure 2.  Forest plot for the pooled relative ratio (RR) and 95% confidence interval (CI) of studies on PM2.5 exposure (per 5 μg/m3 increment) with cognitive impairment. The size of the gray box is positively proportional to the weight assigned to each study, and horizontal lines represent the 95% CI.
jogh-10-010417-F2


We carried out subgroup analysis by the source of outcome, study areas, follow-up duration, and adjustment status for comorbidity and smoking. PM2.5 revealed a statistically significant positive association with the risk of cognitive impairment in population-based studies (RRper 5 μg/m3 = 1.05; 95% CI = 1.01, 1.09; I2 = 57.4%; Pheterogeneity = 0.016), whereas no statistically significant association was found in hospital-based studies (RRper 5 μg/m3 = 1.20; 95% CI = 0.82,1.75; I2 = 97.1%; Pheterogeneity<0.001). Regarding study areas, the RRper 5 μg/m3 were 1.13 (95% CI = 1.01, 1.26; I2 = 86.7%; Pheterogeneity<0.001) for studies conducted in North America, 1.34 (95% CI = 0.94, 1.89; I2 = 73.4%; Pheterogeneity = 0.005) in Europe and 1.10 (95% CI = 0.98, 1.04) in Asia. For follow-up duration, the RRper 5 μg/m3 were statistically significant among the studies with following duration >10 years (RRper 5 μg/m3 = 1.10; 95% CI = 1.03, 1.17; I2 = 86.3%; Pheterogeneity<0.001), while the association was not significant among the studies with following duration ≤10 years (RRper 5 μg/m3 = 1.11; 95% CI = 0.95, 1.31; I2 = 70.4%; Pheterogeneity = 0.018). There was a statistically significant positive relationship between PM2.5 and the risk of cognitive impairment in studies adjusting for comorbidity (RRper 5 μg/m3 = 1.10; 95% CI = 1.03, 1.17; I2 = 85.3%; Pheterogeneity<0.001), while no association was found in studies that did not adjust comorbidity (RRper 5 μg/m3 = 1.07; 95% CI = 0.90, 1.26; I2 = 64.1%; Pheterogeneity = 0.039). The combined RRper 5 μg/m3 (RRper 5 μg/m3 = 1.21; 95% CI = 1.00, 1.46; I2 = 57.0%; Pheterogeneity = 0.040) in smoking-adjusted studies were higher than that (RRper 5 μg/m3 = 1.06; 95% CI = 1.01, 1.11; I2 = 90.2%; Pheterogeneiy<0.001) in studies that did not adjust for smoking.

NO2/NOx exposure and the risk of cognitive impairment

Seven studies [17,18,20,21,25,26] comprising 285 752 cases among 2 589 907 participants were included. The pooled RR of cognitive impairment per 5 ppb increments in exposure to NO2/NOx was 1.02 (95% CI = 0.99, 1.04; I2 = 93.9%; Pheterogeneity<0.001; Figure 3). A positive significant association (RRper 5ppb = 1.03; 95% CI = 1.02, 1.04; I2 = 96.9%; Pheterogeneity<0.001; Fig.S2 in the Online Supplementary Document) of cognitive impairment with NO2/NOx exposure was found after sensitivity analysis with the number of participants as the weight.

Figure 3.  Forest plot for the pooled relative ratio (RR) and 95% confidence interval (CI) of studies on NO2/NOx exposure (per 5 ppb increment) with cognitive impairment. The size of the gray box is positively proportional to the weight assigned to each study, and horizontal lines represent the 95% CI.
jogh-10-010417-F3


We carried out subgroup analysis by the source of outcome, study areas, follow-up duration, and adjustment status for comorbidity and smoking. NO2/NOx revealed a statistically significant positive association with the risk of cognitive impairment in population-based studies (RRper 5 ppb = 1.048; 95% CI = 1.004, 1.094; I2 = 68.5%; Pheterogeneity = 0.013), whereas no association was found in hospital-based studies (RRper 5 ppb = 0.99; 95% CI = 0.95, 1.02; I2 = 94.7%; Pheterogeneity<0.001). Regarding study areas, we didn’t find relationship in either North America (RRper 5 ppb = 1.03; 95% CI = 0.98, 1.08; I2 = 66.4%; Pheterogeneity = 0.051) or Europe (RRper 5 ppb = 1.01; 95% CI = 0.98, 1.05; I2 = 90.1%; Pheterogeneity<0.001). There was no relationship between NO2/NOx and the risk of cognitive impairment both in studies adjusting for comorbidity (RRper 5 ppb= 1.04; 95% CI = 0.99, 1.09; I2 = 72.6%; Pheterogeneity = 0.012) and not adjusting for comorbidity (RRper 5 ppb= 1.00; 95% CI = 0.97, 1.04; I2 = 91.2%; Pheterogeneity<0.001). The combined RRper 5 ppb were statistically significant (RRper 5 ppb = 1.10; 95% CI = 1.02, 1.19; I2 = 53.2%; Pheterogeneity = 0.118) in smoking-adjusted studies, but not in studies that did not adjust for smoking (RRper 5 ppb= 1.00; 95% CI = 0.98, 1.03; I2 = 96.5%; Pheterogeneity<0.001).

O3 exposure and the risk of cognitive impairment

Four studies [17,18,20,22] comprising 282 944 cases among 2 644 151 participants were included. The pooled RR of cognitive impairment per 5 ppb increments in exposure to O3 was 1.00 (95% CI = 0.95,1.06; I2 = 92.9%; Pheterogeneity<0.001; Figure 4). An inverse significant association (RRper 5ppb = 0.98; 95% CI = 0.96, 0.99; I2 = 95.2%; Pheterogeneity<0.001; Fig.S3 in the Online Supplementary Document) of cognitive impairment with O3 exposure was found after sensitivity analysis with the number of participants as the weight.

Figure 4.  Forest plot for the pooled relative ratio (RR) and 95% confidence interval (CI) of studies on O3 exposure (per 5 ppb increment) with cognitive impairment. The size of the gray box is positively proportional to the weight assigned to each study, and horizontal lines represent the 95% CI.
jogh-10-010417-F4


Meta-regression

As displayed in Figure 2, high heterogeneity (I2 = 82.8%, Pheterogeneity = 0.030) was found in the analysis of PM2.5 and the risk of cognitive impairment. Hence, we performed meta-regression with the covariates of the source of outcome (P = 0.663), study areas (P = 0.403), follow-up duration (P = 0.993), whether adjusted comorbidity (P = 0.679) and whether adjusted smoking (P = 0.475) to explore potential sources of the heterogeneity. However, no covariates contributed to the heterogeneity of between-study.

In the analysis of NO2/NOx and the risk of cognitive impairment, as displayed in Figure 3 high heterogeneity (I2 = 93.4%, Pheterogeneity<0.001) was found. The meta-regression with the covariates of the source of outcome (P = 0.219), study areas (P = 0.065), follow-up duration (P = 0.053), whether adjusted comorbidity (P = 0.979) and whether adjusted smoking (P = 0.540) to explore potential sources of the heterogeneity, and no covariates contributed to the heterogeneity.

Sensitivity analysis and influence analysis

For the PM2.5 exposure and the risk of cognitive impairment, we carried out the leave-one-out sensitivity analysis. After excluding two studies [19,20], I2 decreased from 82.2% to 57.4% (P = 0.016), and the result remained significant (RRper 5 μg/m3 = 1.05; 95% CI = 1.01,1.09; Figure S4 in the Online Supplementary Document).

In the influence analysis, no individual study had an overmuch impact on the pooled effect for PM2.5 and NO2/NOx with the risk of cognitive impairment (Figure S5 in the Online Supplementary Document and Figure S6 in the Online Supplementary Document).

Publication bias

The visual scrutiny of the funnel plot (Figure S7 in the Online Supplementary Document and Figure S8 in the Online Supplementary Document) seemed to be asymmetrical for PM2.5 and NO2/NOx, while the Egger’s test displayed no evidence of significant publication bias in the analysis between PM2.5 (P = 0.155) and NO2/NOx (P = 0.792) and the risk of cognitive impairment, respectively.

DISCUSSION

This meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies included 10 articles to quantitatively evaluate the association between air pollution exposure and the risk of cognitive impairment. The results demonstrated that exposure to PM2.5 was significantly related to an increased risk of cognitive impairment. More specifically, for every 5 μg/m3 increase in PM2.5 concentration, the risk of cognitive impairment increased by 8%. Nevertheless, there is no statistical association between NO2/NOx and O3 exposure and the risk of cognitive impairment. And the correlation direction did not change after sensitivity analysis. For PM2.5 exposure, subgroup analysis showed significant associations in population-based studies, studies that conducted in North America and studies with follow-up duration >10 years.

Several biological mechanisms have been put forward for the positive relationship of particulate matter (PM) with cognitive impairment. First, exposure to air pollution, particularly PM might cause neuroinflammation, oxidative stress and change brain innate immune responses in early adulthood [36]. At present, inflammation and oxidative stress have been confirmed as basic mechanisms by which air pollution may affect central nervous system disease (CNS) [9]. Second, PM can activate microglia [37], excessive and chronic activation may lead to neurotoxicity [38]. Importantly, microglial activation has been implicated in the progression of diseases such as dementia [39]. Third, the olfactory bulb is another pathway [40] through which PM enters the body, reaching the brain directly and inducing a series of changes such as increased the level of amyloid-β42, hyperphosphorylated τ, and neural degeneration [41,42].

High heterogeneity appeared in this meta-analysis of PM2.5 and NO2/NOx and the risk of cognitive impairment. To search for potential heterogeneity, we performed the following work. Meta-regression was performed to detect the potential factors that contributed to heterogeneity between studies, however, no factors were found to do with it. We also conducted influence analysis, and the results indicated that no individual study had an excessive impact on the pooled effect of PM2.5 and NO2/NOx and the risk of cognitive impairment. For PM2.5 exposure, the leave-one-out sensitivity analysis indicated that two studies [19,20] affected the heterogeneity and after further excluding two studies, the pooled RR was decreased but not altered substantially. In two studies, the source of outcome was from the hospital-based population, which was likely to not cover all patients and could introduce selection bias.

Our study presents several advantages. First, our meta-analysis included a large number of participants from longitudinal cohort studies, providing high statistical power and making it more likely to obtain a reasonable conclusion. Second, the pooled RR of this meta-analysis was based on data on dose-response relationships in the original studies, thus the existence of causation was further supported. Furthermore, to minimize between-study variation, we normalized the exposure levels of PM2.5, NO2/NOx and O3 across studies into uniform units, and the pooled RR was standardized per 5μg/m3 increments for PM2.5 and 5 ppb for NO2/NOx and O3. Third, original studies were all fully taken into account potential confounders such as age. Moreover, sex, comorbidities, and smoking were adjusted for in most studies. As well, the quality assessment score of each study was higher than 7, demonstrating that the quality of the included articles was generally good.

Nevertheless, several limitations of our meta-analysis should be recognized. First, although as much as possible potential confounders were adjusted for in most studies, several studies still did not adjust for potential confounders, such as comorbidities. Comorbidities include heart disease, stroke, diabetes, hypertension and so on, which may elevate cognitive impairment risk [43-45]. In the analysis of PM2.5 and cognitive impairment, a significant association was found in studies adjusting for comorbidity, but no association was found in studies that did not adjust for comorbidity, therefore the combined result was underestimated. Second, varied exposure assessment methods differed in its ability to provide estimates of individual exposure levels, which might increase the instability of the results to some extent. Third, the definition of cognitive impairment was inconsistent, resulting in misclassification bias.

CONCLUSIONS

In summary, the results of this meta-analysis demonstrated that exposure to PM2.5 was associated with an increase in cognitive impairment risk. The results may have substantial public health significance for the prevention of cognitive impairment through air pollution interventions.

Additional material
Notes

[1] Funding: None.

[2] Author’s contribution: XY and DZ designed the study, participated in its design and coordination, interpretation of the data and involved in drafting the manuscript. XY, DZ and WJ involved in revising it critically for important intellectual content. XY, and LZ performed the bibliographical search, data extraction and interpretation of the data.

[3] Competing interests: The authors completed the Unified Competing Interest form at www.icmje.org/coi_disclosure.pdf (available upon request from the corresponding author), and declare no conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

[1] JH Dickey. Part VII. Air pollution: overview of sources and health effects. Dis Mon. 2000;46:566-89. DOI: 10.1016/S0011-5029(00)90024-5. [PMID:11021547]

[2] World Health Organization. 2019. Ten threats to global health in 2019. Available: https://www.who.int/emergencies/ten-threats-to-global-health-in-2019. 2019. Accessed: 15 October 2019.

[3] World Health Organization. 9 out of 10 people worldwide breathe polluted air, but more countries are taking action. Available: https://www.who.int/news-room/detail/02-05-2018-9-out-of-10-people-worldwide-breathe-polluted-air-but-more-countries-are-taking-action. 2019. Accessed: 15 October 2019.

[4] E Duron and O Hanon. Vascular risk factors, cognitive decline, and dementia. Vasc Health Risk Manag. 2008;4:363-81. DOI: 10.2147/VHRM.S1839. [PMID:18561512]

[5] V Hachinski, C Iadecola, RC Petersen, MM Breteler, DL Nyenhuis, and SE Black. National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke-Canadian Stroke Network vascular cognitive impairment harmonization standards. Stroke. 2006;37:2220-41. DOI: 10.1161/01.STR.0000237236.88823.47. [PMID:16917086]

[6] JC Morris, M Storandt, JP Miller, DW McKeel, JL Price, and EH Rubin. Mild cognitive impairment represents early-stage Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol. 2001;58:397-405. DOI: 10.1001/archneur.58.3.397. [PMID:11255443]

[7] World Health Organization. 2019. The top ten causes of death. Available: https://www.who.int/en/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/the-top-10-causes-of-death. Accessed: 15 October 2019.

[8] . 2019;https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/dementiaAccessed October 15, 2019

[9] ML Block and L Calderon-Garciduenas. Air pollution: mechanisms of neuroinflammation and CNS disease. Trends Neurosci. 2009;32:506-16. DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2009.05.009. [PMID:19716187]

[10] S Genc, Z Zadeoglulari, SH Fuss, and K Genc. The adverse effects of air pollution on the nervous system. J Toxicol. 2012;2012:782462. DOI: 10.1155/2012/782462. [PMID:22523490]

[11] B González-Flecha. Oxidant mechanisms in response to ambient air particles. Mol Aspects Med. 2004;25:169-82. DOI: 10.1016/j.mam.2004.02.017. [PMID:15051325]

[12] DJ Bonda, X Wang, G Perry, A Nunomura, M Tabaton, and X Zhu. Oxidative stress in Alzheimer disease: a possibility for prevention. Neuropharmacology. 2010;59:290-4. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2010.04.005. [PMID:20394761]

[13] JJ Hoozemans, R Veerhuis, JM Rozemuller, and P Eikelenboom. Neuroinflammation and regeneration in the early stages of Alzheimer’s disease pathology. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2006;24:157-65. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2005.11.001. [PMID:16384684]

[14] L Minghetti. Role of inflammation in neurodegenerative diseases. Curr Opin Neurol. 2005;18:315-21. DOI: 10.1097/01.wco.0000169752.54191.97. [PMID:15891419]

[15] B Su, X Wang, A Nunomura, PI Moreira, H-g Lee, and G Perry. Oxidative stress signaling in Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2008;5:525-32. DOI: 10.2174/156720508786898451. [PMID:19075578]

[16] X Zhu, AK Raina, H-g Lee, G Casadesus, MA Smith, and G Perry. Oxidative stress signalling in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain Res. 2004;1000:32-9. DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.01.012. [PMID:15053949]

[17] IM Carey, HR Anderson, RW Atkinson, SD Beevers, DG Cook, and DP Strachan. Are noise and air pollution related to the incidence of dementia? A cohort study in London, England. BMJ Open. 2018;8:e022404. DOI: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-022404. [PMID:30206085]

[18] H Chen, JC Kwong, R Copes, P Hystad, A van Donkelaar, and K Tu. Exposure to ambient air pollution and the incidence of dementia: A population-based cohort study. Environ Int. 2017;108:271-7. DOI: 10.1016/j.envint.2017.08.020. [PMID:28917207]

[19] MA Kioumourtzoglou, JD Schwartz, MG Weisskopf, SJ Melly, Y Wang, and F Dominici. Long-term PM2.5 Exposure and Neurological Hospital Admissions in the Northeastern United States. Environ Health Perspect. 2016;124:23-9. DOI: 10.1289/ehp.1408973. [PMID:25978701]

[20] F Cerza, M Renzi, C Gariazzo, M Davoli, P Michelozzi, and F Forastiere. Long-term exposure to air pollution and hospitalization for dementia in the Rome longitudinal study. Environ Health. 2019;18:72 DOI: 10.1186/s12940-019-0511-5. [PMID:31399053]

[21] SD Ilango, H Chen, P Hystad, A van Donkelaar, JC Kwong, and K Tu. The role of cardiovascular disease in the relationship between air pollution and incident dementia: a population-based cohort study. Int J Epidemiol. 2019;DOI: 10.1093/ije/dyz154. [PMID:31347651]

[22] CR Jung, YT Lin, and BF Hwang. Ozone, particulate matter, and newly diagnosed Alzheimer’s disease: a population-based cohort study in Taiwan. J Alzheimers Dis. 2015;44:573-84. DOI: 10.3233/JAD-140855. [PMID:25310992]

[23] MS Loop, ST Kent, MZ Al-Hamdan, WL Crosson, SM Estes, and MG Estes. Fine particulate matter and incident cognitive impairment in the REasons for Geographic and Racial Differences in Stroke (REGARDS) cohort. PLoS One. 2013;8:e75001. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075001. [PMID:24086422]

[24] A Oudin, D Segersson, R Adolfsson, and B Forsberg. Association between air pollution from residential wood burning and dementia incidence in a longitudinal study in Northern Sweden. PLoS One. 2018;13:e0198283. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0198283. [PMID:29897947]

[25] Yu Y. Associations between long term exposure to air pollution and the incidence of dementia and cognitive impairment in a Canadian prospective cohort study [dissertation]. Carleton University: Carleton University; 2016.

[26] A Oudin, B Forsberg, AN Adolfsson, N Lind, L Modig, and M Nordin. Traffic-Related Air Pollution and Dementia Incidence in Northern Sweden: A Longitudinal Study. Environ Health Perspect. 2016;124:306-12. DOI: 10.1289/ehp.1408322. [PMID:26305859]

[27] D Moher, A Liberati, J Tetzlaff, and DG Altman. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Ann Intern Med. 2009;151:264-9. DOI: 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135. [PMID:19622511]

[28] X Zeng, Y Zhang, JS Kwong, C Zhang, S Li, and F Sun. The methodological quality assessment tools for preclinical and clinical studies, systematic review and meta-analysis, and clinical practice guideline: a systematic review. J Evid Based Med. 2015;8:2-10. DOI: 10.1111/jebm.12141. [PMID:25594108]

[29] NTT Nhung, H Amini, C Schindler, M Kutlar Joss, TM Dien, and N Probst-Hensch. Short-term association between ambient air pollution and pneumonia in children: A systematic review and meta-analysis of time-series and case-crossover studies. Environ Pollut. 2017;230:1000-8. DOI: 10.1016/j.envpol.2017.07.063. [PMID:28763933]

[30] M Vrijheid, D Martinez, S Manzanares, P Dadvand, A Schembari, and J Rankin. Ambient air pollution and risk of congenital anomalies: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Environ Health Perspect. 2011;119:598-606. DOI: 10.1289/ehp.1002946. [PMID:21131253]

[31] JP Higgins and SG Thompson. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat Med. 2002;21:1539-58. DOI: 10.1002/sim.1186. [PMID:12111919]

[32] JP Higgins and SG Thompson. Controlling the risk of spurious findings from meta-regression. Stat Med. 2004;23:1663-82. DOI: 10.1002/sim.1752. [PMID:15160401]

[33] NA Patsopoulos, E Evangelou, and JP Ioannidis. Sensitivity of between-study heterogeneity in meta-analysis: proposed metrics and empirical evaluation. Int J Epidemiol. 2008;37:1148-57. DOI: 10.1093/ije/dyn065. [PMID:18424475]

[34] A Tobias. Assessing the influence of a single study in the meta-analysis estimate. Stata Tech Bull. 1999;47:15-7.

[35] M Egger, G Davey Smith, M Schneider, and C Minder. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315:629-34. DOI: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629. [PMID:9310563]

[36] L Calderón-Garcidueñas, AC Solt, C Henriquez-Roldan, R Torres-Jardon, B Nuse, and L Herritt. Long-term air pollution exposure is associated with neuroinflammation, an altered innate immune response, disruption of the blood-brain barrier, ultrafine particulate deposition, and accumulation of amyloid beta-42 and alpha-synuclein in children and young adults. Toxicol Pathol. 2008;36:289-310. DOI: 10.1177/0192623307313011. [PMID:18349428]

[37] S Levesque, T Taetzsch, ME Lull, U Kodavanti, K Stadler, and A Wagner. Diesel exhaust activates and primes microglia: air pollution, neuroinflammation, and regulation of dopaminergic neurotoxicity. Environ Health Perspect. 2011;119:1149-55. DOI: 10.1289/ehp.1002986. [PMID:21561831]

[38] ML Block and JS Hong. Chronic microglial activation and progressive dopaminergic neurotoxicity. Biochem Soc Trans. 2007;35:1127-32. DOI: 10.1042/BST0351127. [PMID:17956294]

[39] PL McGeer, S Itagaki, BE Boyes, and EG McGeer. Reactive microglia are positive for HLA-DR in the substantia nigra of Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease brains. Neurology. 1988;38:1285-91. DOI: 10.1212/WNL.38.8.1285. [PMID:3399080]

[40] L Calderón-Garcidueñas, M Franco-Lira, C Henriquez-Roldan, N Osnaya, A Gonzalez-Maciel, and R Reynoso-Robles. Urban air pollution: influences on olfactory function and pathology in exposed children and young adults. Exp Toxicol Pathol. 2010;62:91-102. DOI: 10.1016/j.etp.2009.02.117. [PMID:19297138]

[41] Rapid doubling of Alzheimer’s amyloid-β40 and 42 levels in brains of mice exposed to a nickel nanoparticle model of air pollution. F1000 Res. 2012;1:70 DOI: 10.12688/f1000research.1-70.v1

[42] ML Block, A Elder, RL Auten, SD Bilbo, H Chen, and JC Chen. The outdoor air pollution and brain health workshop. Neurotoxicology. 2012;33:972-84. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuro.2012.08.014. [PMID:22981845]

[43] G Cheng, C Huang, H Deng, and H Wang. Diabetes as a risk factor for dementia and mild cognitive impairment: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Intern Med J. 2012;42:484-91. DOI: 10.1111/j.1445-5994.2012.02758.x. [PMID:22372522]

[44] R Sahathevan, A Brodtmann, and GA Donnan. Dementia, stroke, and vascular risk factors; a review. Int J Stroke. 2012;7:61-73. DOI: 10.1111/j.1747-4949.2011.00731.x. [PMID:22188853]

[45] SI Sharp, D Aarsland, S Day, H Sonnesyn, and C Ballard. Hypertension is a potential risk factor for vascular dementia: systematic review. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2011;26:661-9. DOI: 10.1002/gps.2572. [PMID:21495075]



Journal of Global Health (ISSN 2047-2986), Edinburgh University Global Health Society
Designed by