Journal of Global Health: Home Journal of Global Health
Watch: Survival: The Story of Global Health - FREE
JoGH Recommends:

Peer Review Conference



Creative Commons Licence
This work is licensed under a
Creative Commons Attribution
4.0 International License
.


Andrew Gibbs, Kristin Dunkle, and Rachel Jewkes

Abstract

Background

Emotional and economic intimate partner violence (IPV) are common. There remain outstanding questions: 1) whether the patterning of emotional and economic IPV varies across contexts, and whether the current indicators adequately capture this variation; 2) whether simply binary or more complex modelling strategies are appropriate; 3) whether health impacts of emotional and economic IPV are sustained in population-based studies, across multiple settings.

Methods

Ever partnered women (18-49 years) in cross-sectional, population-based data from three countries, China, Papua New Guinea (PNG) and Sri Lanka, from the United Nations Multi-country Study on Men and Violence in Asia and the Pacific. We assessed lifetime experience of emotional IPV (5 items) and economic IPV (4 items), item uniqueness (ie, the extent to which a person only reported that item), and descriptive associations and multivariable regression between combinations of emotional and economic IPV and physical and/or sexual IPV, for depressive symptoms and generalized health.

Results

In all countries, lifetime emotional and economic IPV were common. By item, only one emotional IPV item (he hurt others of importance) had <3% of women uniquely identified by it. There was no item with low uniqueness for economic IPV. By item, and the entire scale, two or more experiences of emotional IPV, or economic IPV, were consistently associated with worse depression and generalized health. Emotional IPV was independently associated with higher depressive scores, and emotional IPV was independently associated with worse generalised health scores, across multiple models. Women experiencing physical and/or sexual IPV combined with emotional or economic IPV, reported the highest depressive symptoms and worst generalised health scores.

Conclusions

Emotional IPV and economic IPV are more, or as, common as physical IPV and sexual IPV in three countries in Asia-Pacific. The current set of emotional and economic items captures a range of unique instances of IPV and that forms of emotional and economic IPV are patterned across different contexts. In addition, the use of a simple binary coding of these scales provides a robust way of providing a measure of health impact. The simplicity of this approach enables replication and standardization of measurement of these key constructs across multiple settings, enabling comparison.


Intimate partner violence (IPV) is a global public health problem [1]; the fifth Sustainable Development Goal includes a specific target and indicators to eliminate IPV [2]. While physical IPV and sexual IPV have received much attention, the scale and negative health impacts of women’s experiences of emotional and economic IPV are increasingly recognised [3]. Physical IPV refers to physically hurting a current or previous intimate partner, sexual IPV to coerced or physically forced sexual contact with a current or previous partner [4]. Emotional IPV includes insults and verbal abuse, as well as threats to do harm to a current or former partner or a member of their family, or pets (para-social violence) [5]. While economic IPV, refers to control of a current or former partner’s resources or seeking to limit their economic activities, and also includes throwing people out of their home, and not providing economic support to a family, when there are resources to do so [4].

Studies often show emotional IPV is the most common form of IPV women experience. For instance in an analysis of 12 Demographic Health Surveillance (DHS) surveys, from 10 African countries, lifetime emotional IPV ranged from 35.9% in Liberia, to 9.4% in Mali, and was typically the most common form of IPV [6]. However, population-based estimates of economic IPV experience are less common. In Australia a population-based sample estimated lifetime economic IPV to be 15.7% [7], while in Palestine, lifetime economic IPV was estimated to 45% [8]. Despite growing evidence about the frequency with which women experience emotional IPV, and economic IPV, there remains very little comparative population-based data from multiple countries, looking at all four forms of IPV in consistent ways.

Questions also remain as to whether emotional and economic IPV are patterned the same across different countries/contexts. In qualitative research on emotional IPV in Rwanda and South Africa, Stern et al [9] demonstrated that while there were differences in specific ways women experienced emotional IPV, linked to different norms around relationships and women’s mobility, the broad forms women experienced were remarkably similar. It is likely that women’s experiences of economic IPV are similarly varied, shaped by different work opportunities, norms about women’s work, and the levels of poverty. Understanding the patterning of individual indicators commonly used in emotional and economic IPV scales, may help us understand these differences, and whether there are any redundant items, which could be removed to reduce interviewee fatigue.

There is also evidence that emotional IPV and economic IPV have negative health impacts, independent from physical and sexual IPV. Studies have linked emotional IPV to post-natal depression [10], depressive symptomology [11,12] suicidal ideation [13-15], and psychosomatic conditions [12]. Similarly, despite less research, the negative mental health impacts of economic IPV have also been described, including higher depressive symptoms [8,11], suicidal ideation [11], and anxiety [8].

While evidence on the potential negative health impacts of emotional and economic IPV is growing, it remains limited. Much of the research on this topic has used small, non-representative samples and/or failed to account for co-occurrence of physical and/or sexual IPV. Thus, important questions remain regarding the extent to which emotional and economic IPV are independent risk factors for adverse health outcomes, as well as the extent to which their impact is obscured or potentially magnified when they co-occur with physical and/or sexual IPV. There is also not yet an agreed standard for the correct measurement of emotional or economic IPV, the items/constructs to be included and whether these phenomenon are best-modelled using simple binary approaches, or more complex scoring to demonstrate health impacts.

This paper draws on population-based data from three countries in the Asia-Pacific region to address four objectives. First, to estimate the prevalence of lifetime experience of emotional IPV and economic IPV in these populations. Second, to describe the lifetime prevalence of individual items within each scale, and assess their unique contribution to the scale, by country. Third, to assess whether particular items from a scale are more strongly associated with adverse health impacts. Fourth, to assess whether emotional IPV and economic IPV make independent contributions to self-reported health outcomes while accounting for co-occurrence of physical and/or sexual IPV.

METHODS

The study was conceptualized by Partners for Prevention (P4P), with support from the South African Medical Research Council and country research teams. The overall P4P study was designed to assess men’s perpetration of IPV in six countries. Four of these sites also sampled women, and were considered for inclusion here. However only three of them included measures of health outcomes for female participants. Those were Sri Lanka, China and Papua New Guinea (PNG).

Ethics

Ethical approval for the research was provided by the Medical Research Council of South Africa; the College of Humanities, Beijing Forestry University; and the Faculty of Medicine at the University of Colombo, Sri Lanka [16]. We followed ethical and safety guidelines for research on violence against women [4]. Interviewees received an information sheet and provided written consent.

Sample

Data collection sites varied. In Sri Lanka, the city of Colombo and three contrasting districts were study sites. In China, research took place in a single county, with sampling split between a rural area and town. In PNG, there was a representative sample of the island of Bougainville. Further details of the research can be found elsewhere [16,17].

In each site, we selected census enumeration areas, with a probability proportionate to size, and systematically selected households within these areas. In households, we invited a woman aged 18–49 years (where necessary, randomly selected) for a face-to-face interview, with a trained female interviewer). In China, a household list of individuals in each cluster by age and sex was available and used for sampling within selected clusters, and the entire questionnaire was self-completed. Full details of the methods, sampling, are presented elsewhere [16,17]. The proportion of enumerated and eligible women interviewed per site varied, in Sri Lanka it was 73.9%, China 84.9%, and PNG 85.0% [16,17].

Measures

Health outcomes

Two health outcomes were assessed across the countries, depression and self-rated health. In China and PNG depressive symptomology was assessed through ten items drawn from the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CESD) Scale [18]. Each item asked about clinically relevant symptoms of depression in the past week, with responses, ‘rarely or never, some or a little, moderate amount of time, most or all the time’. Scores were summed directly with higher scores indicating greater symptoms of depression (range 0-30; Cronbach α = 0.86 China, α = 0.89 PNG). Given that the CESD10 is a screening tool and not a method for diagnosing depression it was treated as a score in analyses, but for descriptive purposes a cut of 8 or more was used as more indicative of high levels of depressive symptomology [19]. The CESD was not asked in Sri Lanka, based on a decision by the country-team.

Self-rated generalised health was assessed in all three countries through a single item: “In general, would you describe your overall health as excellent, good, fair, poor or very poor?” Responses were on a five-point scale, from excellent to very poor. Higher scores indicated worse health.

Intimate partner violence measures

The different forms of IPV women may have experienced were assessed through standardized measures in all three countries, based on questions first developed for the WHO multi-country survey on women’s health [3], and only asked to women who reported ever being partnered in their lifetime. These items have been refined in multiple studies globally and previously used in the Asia-Pacific context [3]. To assess emotional IPV, we asked women five questions about their lifetime experiences, for instance: “Has a current or previous husband or boyfriend ever insulted you or made you feel bad about yourself?” Responses were ‘never, once, few, or many times’. Similarly, four items asked about women’s lifetime experiences of economic IPV. An example question was: “Has a current or previous husband or boyfriend ever taken your earnings from you?” Responses were ‘never’, ‘once’, ‘few’, or ‘many times’.

Lifetime experience of physical IPV and sexual IPV were assessed through five and four, behaviourally specific items, respectively. An example of physical IPV was: “Has a current or previous husband or boyfriend ever hit you with a fist or with something else which could hurt you?”, while for sexual IPV: “Has a current or previous husband or boyfriend ever physically forced you to have sex when you did not want to?” Response options for both scales were “never, once, few, or many times”.

All three countries included a series of standardized questions about socio-demographics, including age, highest level of education, current relationship status, food-security and ability to mobilise resources (cash) in an emergency. We also asked about nine experiences of childhood abuse and neglect, using items adapted from the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire.

Analysis

The country data sets were combined into one. Data analysis were conducted using Stata IC/14.1 (StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA). Procedures accounted for the multistage structure of the data set, with stratification by data collection site within countries and enumeration areas as clusters. The data set was self-weighting.

First, we describe the sample by country, and as a pooled data set, using percentages and means as appropriate. We then examine the prevalence of each type of IPV assessed (emotional, economic, physical, sexual), through creating a three-level categorical variable to indicate lifetime experience of IPV: never, once, more than once. We then created an overall IPV experience variable, which parsed out all possible experiences of IPV, to look at overlaps and co-occurrence. This is also presented as a proportional Venn diagram. Descriptive analyses took into account the clustered nature of data, and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were calculated using Taylor linearization [20], to account for data clustering.

Second, to describe prevalence and patterning of emotional IPV and economic IPV by item we: i) provide individual item responses as a percentage by country; and ii) estimate the item’s uniqueness, as the proportion of women who only reported that item in the scale. Essentially identifying the proportion that would not be categorised as experiencing that form of IPV if the item were not asked.

To assess associations between emotional IPV and economic IPV and health outcomes by individual items, and by country, we report the prevalence of each item by i) lifetime never/ever, ii) none or once, compared to few or more lifetime experiences, and iii) lifetime frequency of exposure (never, once, few, many). We present mean scores, and 95% CIs for depressive symptoms (China and PNG), and mean scores and 95% CIs for self-rated health (all countries). Where there is no overlap between 95% CIs, we consider this indication of the mean scores being different.

To assess whether emotional and economic IPV have independent associations with health outcomes, we pooled the data from all countries; country level analysis was infeasible because of sample size issues. We first assess this descriptively, through creating four-level categorical variables for multiple combinations of emotional and physical and/or sexual IPV, and economic and physical and/or sexual IPV. For instance, we created a four-level variable describing women who had i) no exposure to IPV ii) only exposure to economic IPV in their lifetime, iii) exposure to economic IPV and physical IPV in their lifetime, and iv) only exposure to physical IPV in their lifetime. We created a range of these four-level variables, based on frequency of exposure categories (never vs ever, and never or once, vs few or many) and types of exposure (sexual, physical, physical and/or sexual). For each permutation, we provide mean scores and 95% CI for depressive symptoms and self-reported health. We compare whether there is overlap between 95% CIs of each category and those reporting no IPV experience.

We then use unadjusted and adjusted multivariable regression models to assess whether IPV was independently associated with the health outcome in multiple ways. For depression, we used a negative binomial regression, and for self-rated health, ordered logistic regression. First, we separately model the impact of emotional, economic, and physical and/or sexual IPV, with no allowance for co-occurrence (Models 1-3), and then consider them together in a single model (Model 4). However, given that the constructs are largely overlapping, there was significant collinearity. We thus constructed four-level categorical variables comparing combinations of emotional IPV and physical and/or sexual IPV, and economic IPV and physical and/or sexual IPV experiences and health outcomes (Models 6-8). Finally, we created an eight level variable, which categorized women into all possible IPV combinations across the types of IPV its association with health (Models 9 and 10). All adjusted models included age, education, poverty and childhood traumas. All models included adjustment for the survey structure. We present the results of all of these models to explore the extent to which the measurement strategy impacts the point estimates for effect sizes and the conclusions drawn.

RESULTS

Description of the sample

In total 2438 ever partnered women were interviewed across the three countries (Sri Lanka n = 559; PNG n = 792; China n = 1087). In all countries (Table 1), the largest proportion of women were aged 35-49 years old (49.5% of the overall sample). In Sri Lanka and China over half the women interviewed (53.9% and 57.5% respectively) had some secondary education, but only 13.4% in PNG. Very few had no education whatsoever.

Table 1.  Descriptive statistics and intimate partner violence experience per country, and combined
Sri Lanka China PNG Combined
n %/mean (95% CI) n %/mean (95% CI) n %/mean(95% CI) n %/mean (95% CI)
Age (years):
18-24 96 17.2 (14.1-20.7) 122 11.3 (9.6-13.2) 165 20.8 (17.9-24.1) 383 15.7 (14.3-17.3)
25-34 214 38.3 (32.7-44.2) 328 30.3 (27.0-33.9) 303 38.3 (35.1-41.6) 845 34.7 (32.5-37.0)
35-49 249 44.5 (38.7-50.6) 632 58.4 (54.7-62.1) 324 40.9 (37.0-45.0) 1205 49.5 (47.0-52.1)
Education:
None 35 6.3 (4.3-9.1) 10 0.9 (0.5-1.8) 47 5.9 (4.4-8.0) 92 3.8 (3.0-4.8)
Incomplete primary 23 4.1 (2.5-6.7) 98 9.1 (7.1-11.5) 205 25.9 (22.6-29.4) 326 13.4 (11.6-15.4)
Complete primary 35 6.3 (4.0-9.6) 153 14.1 (11.9-16.7) 208 26.3 (22.9-29.9) 396 16.3 (14.4-18.3)
Incomplete secondary 301 53.9 (48.1-59.6) 622 57.5 (54.3-60.6) 106 13.4 (10.7-16.7) 1029 42.3 (39.3-45.4)
Complete secondary/higher 164 29.4 (24.3-35.1) 199 18.4 (15.9-21.1) 226 28.5 (24.7-32.7) 589 24.2 (22.2-26.4)
Relationship status:
Married 503 90.0 (86.8-92.5) 1004 92.8 (91.0-94.2) 608 76.8 (73.3-79.9) 2115 86.9 (85.2-88.5)
Intimate partner violence experience:
Ever emotional IPV:
No experience 366 70.5 (66.0-74.7) 631 62.3 (58.9-65.5) 243 31.0 (27.8-34.4) 1240 53.5 (51.0-56.0)
Once 30 5.8 (4.1-8.1) 129 12.7 (10.8-14.9) 35 4.5 (3.2-6.1) 194 8.4 (7.2-9.7)
Two or more 123 23.7 (20.1-27.7) 253 25.0 (22.2-28.0) 507 64.6 (60.8-68.2) 883 38.1 (35.5-40.8)
Ever economic IPV:
No experience 373 73.0 (64.0-80.4) 755 75.3 (73.0-77.4) 350 44.6 (39.8-49.5) 1478 64.3 (61.4-67.1)
Once 24 4.7 (2.9-7.5) 73 7.3 (6.0-8.9) 44 5.6 (4.3-7.3) 141 6.1 (5.3-7.1)
Two or more 114 22.3 (15.2-31.6) 175 17.5 (15.5-19.6) 391 49.8 (44.7-54.9) 680 29.6 (26.8-32.6)
Ever emotional and/or economic IPV:
No experience 309 61.3 (53.1-68.9) 535 53.7 (50.4-57.0) 170 21.7 (18.7-25.0) 1014 44.4 (41.7-47.2)
Once 32 6.3 (4.5-8.9) 125 12.6 (10.6-14.8) 34 4.3 (2.9-6.5) 191 8.4 (7.2-9.7)
Two or more 163 32.3 (25.0-40.7) 336 33.7 (30.8-36.9) 580 74.0 (70.1-77.6) 1079 47.2 (44.3-50.2)
Ever physical IPV:
No experience 418 79.5 (75.5-82.9) 665 65.0 (61.4-68.5) 381 48.5 (44.1-53.0) 1464 62.7 (60.1-65.3)
Once 23 4.4 (3.0-6.4) 113 11.1 (9.0-13.5) 52 6.6 (5.0-8.6) 188 8.1 (6.9-9.4)
Two or more 85 16.2 (12.5-20.7) 245 24.0 (21.3-26.8) 352 44.8 (40.8-48.9) 682 29.2 (27.0-31.6)
Ever sexual IPV:
No experience 426 83.0 (78.2-87.0) 859 86.0 (83.3-88.3) 328 41.8 (37.5-46.2) 1613 70.2 (67.2-73.1)
Once 17 3.3 (2.1-5.2) 60 6.0 (4.3-8.4) 25 3.2 (2.2-4.5) 102 4.4 (3.5-5.6)
Two or more 70 13.7 (10.2-18.0) 80 8.0 (6.5-9.8) 432 55.0 (50.7-59.3) 582 25.3 (22.6-28.3)
Ever physical and/or sexual IPV:
No experience 363 71.6 (66.5-76.2) 589 59.3 (54.8-63.6) 249 31.8 (28.1-35.6) 1201 52.6 (49.6-55.5)
Once 28 5.5 (3.7-8.3) 125 12.6 (10.4-15.1) 35 4.5 (3.2-6.3) 188 8.2 (7.0-9.7)
Two or more 116 22.9 (18.4-28.1) 280 28.2 (25.1-31.4) 500 63.8 (59.9-67.5) 896 39.2 (36.5-42.0)
All IPV experiences:
None 265 54.1 (46.7-61.3) 405 41.6 (37.6-45.8) 133 17.0 (14.1-20.3) 803 35.8 (33.1-38.5)
Emotional only 28 5.7 (3.6-9.0) 82 8.4 (7.0-10.1) 47 6.0 (4.6-7.9) 157 7.0 (6.0-8.1)
Economic only 35 7.1 (4.5-11.2) 57 5.9 (4.3-7.9) 42 5.4 (3.7-7.7) 134 6.0 (4.8-7.3)
Emotional and economic only 22 4.5 (2.1-9.2) 33 3.4 (2.4-4.7) 26 3.3 (2.2-4.9) 81 3.6 (2.7-4.7)
Emotional and physical/sexual only 29 5.9 (3.7-9.3) 128 13.2 (11.2-15.5) 132 16.9 (14.3-19.8) 289 12.9 (11.4-14.5)
Economic and physical/sexual only 12 2.4 (1.1-5.3) 24 2.5 (1.7-3.6) 30 3.8 (2.5-5.8) 66 2.9 (2.2-3.8)
Physical/sexual only 36 7.3 (4.8-11.1) 118 12.1 (10.0-14.6) 37 4.7 (3.3-6.7) 191 8.5 (7.2-10.0)
All forms of IPV 63 12.9 (9.9-16.5) 126 13.0 (11.2-14.9) 336 42.9 (38.5-37.5) 525 23.4 (21.2-25.8)
Health outcomes:
Depression (mean) 982 4.4 (4.0-4.8) 780 8.1 (7.3-8.9) 1742 10.5 (10.0-11.1)
Depression (8 or more) 264 27.4 (24.4-30.7) 393 50.4 (44.4-56.4) 657 37.7 (34.4-41.2)
Self-perceived health (mean) 532 2.0 (1.8-2.2) 1031 2.0 (2.0-2.1) 782 2.5 (2.4-2.7) 2345 2.2 (2.1-2.3)

CI – confidence interval, PNG – Papau New Guinea, IPV – intimate partner violence



In all countries, lifetime emotional IPV was the most prevalent form of IPV women reported (Table 1), but with wide variation from 29.5% in Sri Lanka to 61.0% in PNG. Overwhelmingly, women who reported emotional IPV were likely to report either “few” or “many” episodes over their lifetime, rather than just “once”.

Lifetime economic IPV was reported by 35.7% of the overall sample, with variation by country (Sri Lanka 26.3%; China 24.8%; PNG 55.4% - Table 1). In Sri Lanka and PNG, economic IPV was as common as physical IPV, while in China it was less common than physical IPV, but more common than sexual IPV. Women who reported any exposure to economic IPV, mainly reported experiencing either “few” or “many” experiences, rather than just “once” in their lifetime.

When all forms of lifetime IPV were considered together and parsed out (Table 1) 35.8% reported never experiencing any IPV, 7.0% only emotional IPV, 6.0% only economic IPV, and 3.6% emotional and economic IPV only. A quarter (23.4%) reported lifetime experience of all forms of IPV. The Proportional Venn diagram (Figure 1) demonstrates these overlaps clearly.

Figure 1.  Proportional Venn diagram demonstrating overlaps between different forms of IPV women experienced in the three countries (pooled data).
jogh-10-010415-F1


Variation in reporting of items by country

Table 2 shows the prevalence of each individual item for emotional IPV. The most common emotional IPV item varied by country: in PNG it was ‘being insulted’ (55.4%), while in Sri Lanka and China it was ‘being scared or intimidated’ (22.1% and 23.8% respectively). In all countries, ‘hurt people you care about’ was the least reported item.

Table 2.  Comparison by item and overall “coverage” and unique identification
Emotional IPV Overall emotional violence % (n) Item % (n) Unique identification % (n)
Has a current or previous husband or boyfriend ever insulted you or made you feel bad about yourself? Sri Lanka 29.5 (153) 14.8 (78) 4.6 (7)
China 37.7 (382) 20.1 (206) 15.4 (59)
PNG 69.0 (542) 55.2 (434) 13.3 (72)
Has a current or previous husband or boyfriend ever belittled or humiliated you in front of other people? Sri Lanka 29.5 (153) 11.4 (60) 1.3 (2)
China 37.7 (382) 15.3 (157) 5.2 (20)
PNG 69.0 (542) 36.2 (284) 1.5 (8)
Has a current or previous husband or boyfriend ever done things to scare or intimidate you on purpose for example, by the way he looked at you, by yelling or smashing things? Sri Lanka 29.5 (153) 22.1 (116) 24.2 (37)
China 37.7 (382) 23.8 (244) 18.8 (72)
PNG 69.0 (542) 42.9 (337) 3.0 (16)
Has a current or previous husband or boyfriend ever threatened to hurt you? Sri Lanka 29.5 (153) 14.3 (75) 7.2 (11)
China 37.7 (382) 11.0 (113) 3.4 (13)
PNG 69.0 (542) 42.4 (333) 2.6 (14)
Has a current or previous husband or boyfriend ever hurt people you care about as a way of hurting you, or damaged things of importance to you? Sri Lanka 29.5 (153) 10.6 (56) 2.6 (4)
China 37.7 (382) 8.2 (84) 2.4 (9)
PNG 69.0 (542) 26.4 (207) 2.2 (12)
Economic IPV Overall economic violence % (n) Item % (n) Unique identification % (n)
Has a current or previous husband or boyfriend ever prohibited you from getting a job, going to work, trading, earning money or participating in income generation projects? Sri Lanka 27.1 (138) 16.7 (88) 39.1 (54)
China 24.7 (248) 14.4 (147) 43.5 (108)
PNG 55.4 (435) 21.0 (165) 5.1 (22)
Has a current or previous husband or boyfriend ever taken your earnings from you? Sri Lanka 27.1 (138) 13.0 (68) 21.0 (29)
China 24.7 (248) 9.5 (97) 22.6 (56)
PNG 55.4 (435) 35.0 (275) 22.5 (98)
Has a current or previous husband or boyfriend ever thrown you or your children out of the house where you were living? Sri Lanka 27.1 (138) 2.5 (13) 0.7 (1)
China 24.7 (248) 4.3 (44) 4.8 (12)
PNG 55.4 (435) 21.9 (172) 6.0 (26)
Has a current or previous husband or boyfriend ever refused to give you money you needed for household expenses even when he has money for other things? Sri Lanka 27.1 (138) 6.4 (33) 7.2 (10)
China 24.7 (248) 4.7 (48) 5.2 (13)
PNG 55.4 (435) 28.0 (220) 12.6 (55)


The extent to which women only reported that item for emotional IPV – uniqueness – varied by country. In all countries, the item “hurt others” was uniquely reported by <3% of participants, but there was no other item showing such consistency.

For economic IPV (Table 2) the most prevalent item varied across country; in PNG ‘earnings taken’ was reported by a third (35.0%) of women, while in Sri Lanka and China the most common form was “prohibited from getting a job” (16.7%; 14.4% respectively). In all three countries, the least common form of economic IPV reported was “thrown out of house” (2.5% Sri Lanka; 4.3% China; 21.9% PNG).

For economic IPV there was no clear patterning for uniqueness of responses by country. ‘Prohibited from working’ had high unique percentages in Sri Lanka and China (39.1% and 43.5% respectively), but not PNG (5.1%). While for a fifth of women in all three countries the only form of economic IPV they reported was ‘having earnings taken’ (21.0% Sri Lanka; 22.6% China; 22.5% PNG). Overall, the lowest unique item in all three countries was “thrown out of house” where uniqueness ranged from 0.7% (Sri Lanka) to 6.0% (PNG).

Descriptive association between emotional IPV items and health outcomes

Descriptive associations between each emotional IPV scale item and health symptom mean scores by country were assessed (Table S1 in the Online Supplementary Document). For individual items of emotional IPV, there was a consistent patterning for the never/ever binary, and never or once vs ‘few or many’ binary, and their association with health impacts, with women reporting experiencing these forms of emotional IPV having higher means scores of depressive symptoms and worse self-reported health compared to those reporting no experience. For depressive symptomology, there was no overlap between 95% CI for any item, and for self-rated health there was no overlap between 95% CIs in 14/30 instances.

By the frequency of reported lifetime experience (never, once, few, many) for individual emotional IPV items (Table S1 in the Online Supplementary Document), there was a consistent gradation, whereby women reporting more experiences of any item, reported higher mean depressive symptoms. For all items of the emotional IPV scale, women reporting ‘few’ or ‘many’ experiences had higher mean depressive symptoms, with no overlap of 95% CIs, compared to those reporting “never”.

For self-reported health, frequency of experience by emotional IPV item was associated with worse health, compared to those reporting no experience (Table S1 in the Online Supplementary Document). Only with one item “scare or intimidate you” was there consistency across countries, whereby in all three countries those reporting ‘many’ experiences had no overlap with “never” for 95% CIs. For “insulted you” there was no overlap between 95% CIs for China and PNG, among women reporting “many” experiences of this, compared to “never”.

Descriptive association between economic IPV items and health outcomes

Individual items of the economic IPV scale were assessed by health outcomes. For each item (Table S2 in the Online Supplementary Document), there was a similar patterning to emotional IPV, whereby higher mean scores were reported by those reporting more experiences of economic IPV. For depressive symptoms for never/ever, and never or once/two or more experiences, there was no overlap between 95% CIs for 13/16 combinations. For self-rated health, similarly the majority of binary combinations (17/24) had no overlap of 95% CIs.

For the frequency of experience of individual economic IPV items (Table S2 in the Online Supplementary Document), those reporting “once”, “few” or “many” experiences for each item had higher mean scores for depressive symptoms and self-rated health, compared to those reporting “never”. However, there was little consistency, whereby in 4/8 combinations for depressive symptoms women reporting ‘few’ had higher depressive scores than those reporting ‘many’ experiences.

Descriptive and adjusted associations between different combinations of IPV and health outcomes

We also assessed descriptive (Table S3 in the Online Supplementary Document), unadjusted and adjusted associations (Table 3, Table 4 and Table 5) between different IPV combinations and health outcomes. For emotional IPV there are relatively consistent patterns. In all different models (descriptive and regression) emotional IPV is associated with more depressive symptoms, whether emotional IPV was coded as one or more experience, or two or more experiences. However, there is less clear patterning between emotional IPV and self-rated health; descriptively (Table S3 in the Online Supplementary Document) in 2/6 cases there is overlap between 95% CIs for emotional IPV only, and no IPV experience. In adjusted regressions in 3/6 models (Models 6, 9 and 10 in Table 4 and Table 5) emotional IPV was not associated with self-rated health.

Table 3.  Unadjusted and adjusted associations between IPV experiences and health outcomes*
Depression† Self-rated health‡
Unadjusted Adjusted§ Unadjusted Adjusted*
β (95% CI) P-value β (95% CI) P-value β (95% CI) P-value β (95% CI) P-value
Model 1: Emotional IPV (1 or more) 0.63 (0.52, 0.73) <0.001 0.37 (0.26, 0.47) <0.001 0.80 (0.63, 0.97) <0.001 0.47 (0.29, 0.66) <0.001
Model 2: Economic IPV (1 or more) 0.53 (0.42, 0.65) <0.001 0.27 (0.15, 0.38) <0.001 0.75 (0.55, 0.94) <0.001 0.45 (0.26, 0.65) <0.001
Model 3: Physical and/or sexual IPV (1 or more) 0.63 (0.52, 0.74) <0.001 0.37 (0.26, 0.48) <0.001 0.75 (0.57, 0.93) <0.001 0.44 (0.25, 0.63) <0.001
Model 4:
Emotional IPV (1 or more) 0.32 (0.19, 0.45) <0.001 0.21 (0.08, 0.33) 0.001 0.44 (0.23, 0.65) <0.001 0.27 (0.05, 0.50) 0.017
Economic IPV (1 or more) 0.26 (0.15, 0.37) <0.001 0.12 (0.00, 0.23) 0.05 0.42 (0.18, 0.65) 0.001 0.28 (0.05, 0.51) 0.016
Physical and/or sexual IPV (1 or more) 0.36 (0.23, 0.48) <0.001 0.24 (0.11, 0.37) <0.001 0.36 (0.12, 0.59) 0.003 0.24 (0.01, 0.47) 0.045

*Models 1-3: Independent association between individual IPV predictors and health outcome (not adjusting for other IPV experiences); Model 4: Independent association between IPV experiences and health outcomes, adjusting for other IPV experiences.

†Negative binomial regression.

‡Ordered logistic regression.

§Adjusted for: age, education, poverty and childhood traumas.



Table 4.  Unadjusted and adjusted associations between different IPV combinations and health outcomes*
Depression† Self-rated health‡
Unadjusted Adjusted§ Unadjusted Adjusted§
β (95% CI) P-value β (95% CI) P-value β (95% CI) P-value β (95% CI) P-value
Model 5: None ref ref ref ref
Emotional only (1 or more) 0.37 (0.20, 0.54) <0.001 0.31 (0.15, 0.47) <0.001 0.43 (0.15, 0.70) 0.003 0.28 (0.01, 0.55) 0.04
Emotional (1 or more) and p/s (1 or more) 0.81 (0.69, 0.93) <0.001 0.50 (0.39, 0.62) <0.001 1.00 (0.79, 1.20) <0.001 0.61 (0.39 0.84) <0.001
Physical and/or sexual only (1 or more) 0.39 (0.23, 0.55) <0.001 0.32 (0.17, 0.48) <0.001 0.32 (0.06, 0.58) 0.015 0.23 (-0.04, 0.51) 0.093
Model 6:
None ref ref ref ref
Emotional (2 or more) 0.46 (0.28, 0.64) <0.001 0.35 (0.17, 0.53) <0.001 0.31 (0.03, 0.59) 0.033 0.14 (-0.15, 0.42) 0.342
Emotional (2 or more) and p/s (2 or more) 0.83 (0.72, 0.94) <0.001 0.48 (0.36, 0.60) <0.001 1.05 (0.82, 1.28) <0.001 0.61 (0.37, 0.86) <0.001
Physical and/or sexual only (2 or more) 0.51 (0.36, 0.66) <0.001 0.41 (0.27, 0.54) <0.001 0.51 (0.20, 0.82) 0.002 0.38 (0.06, 0.71) 0.02
Model 7:
None ref ref ref ref
Economic only (1 or more) 0.25 (0.06, 0.44) 0.009 0.12 (-0.07, 0.32) 0.203 0.41 (0.05, 0.78) 0.028 0.30 (-0.03, 0.64) 0.078
Economic (1 or more) and p/s (1 or more) 0.84 (0.70, 0.97) <0.001 0.49 (0.34, 0.63) <0.001 1.10 (0.88, 1.32) <0.001 0.70 (0.49, 0.92) <0.001
Physical and/or sexual only (1 or more) 0.45 (0.34, 0.57) <0.001 0.30 (0.19, 0.42) <0.001 0.49 (0.27, 0.72) <0.001 0.34 (0.11, 0.57) 0.005
Model 8:
None ref ref ref ref
Economic (2 or more) 0.45 (0.27, 0.62) <0.001 0.28 (0.11, 0.46) 0.002 0.62 (0.26, 0.99) 0.001 0.46 (0.12, 0.80) 0.008
Economic (2 or more) and p/s (2 or more) 0.87 (0.75, 0.98) <0.001 0.50 (0.36, 0.64) <0.001 1.21 (0.95, 1.46) <0.001 0.79 (0.53, 1.05) <0.001
Physical and/or sexual only (2 or more) 0.57 (0.46, 0.68) <0.001 0.37 (0.27, 0.48) <0.001 0.67 (0.40, 0.94) <0.001 0.46 (0.18, 0.73) 0.001

*Models 5-8: Different IPV combinations experienced by women, disregarding IPV experience not mentioned.

†Negative binomial regression.

‡Ordered logistic regression.

§Adjusted for: age, education, poverty and childhood traumas.



Table 5.  Adjusted and unadjusted associations between depression and IPV experience, and self-reported health and IPV experience*
Depression* Self-rated health†
Unadjusted Adjusted‡ Unadjusted Adjusted‡
β (95% CI) P-value β (95% CI) P-value β (95% CI) P-value β (95% CI) P-value
Model 9:
None ref ref ref ref
Emotional only 0.24 (0.05, 0.43) 0.016 0.21 (0.02, 0.40) 0.032 0.370.07, 0.66) 0.017 0.26 (-0.05, 0.56) 0.1
Economic only 0.05 (-0.23, 0.33) 0.724 -0.05 (-0.29, 0.19) 0.676 0.31 (-0.06, 0.68) 0.103 0.26 (-0.09, 0.61) 0.147
Emotional and economic only 0.62 (0.36, 0.88) <0.001 0.46 (0.19, 0.73) 0.001 0.70 (0.12, 1.28) 0.018 0.49 (-0.01, 1.00) 0.056
Emotional and physical/sexual only 0.61 (0.48, 0.73) <0.001 0.41 (0.29, 0.52) <0.001 0.71 (0.41, 1.02) <0.001 0.49 (0.17, 0.80) 0.003
Economic and physical/sexual only 0.59 (0.33, 0.85) <0.001 0.44 (0.19, 0.70) 0.001 0.58 (0.15, 1.01) 0.009 0.47 (0.02, 0.92) 0.04
Physical/Sexual only 0.31 (0.10, 0.52) 0.004 0.26 (0.06, 0.46) 0.012 0.29 (0.00, 0.59) 0.053 0.22 (-0.10, 0.53) 0.18
All IPV experienced 0.92 (0.78, 1.06) <0.001 0.55 (0.40, 0.70) <0.001 1.24 (1.00, 1.47) <0.001 0.80 (0.56, 1.04) <0.001
Model 10:
None ref ref ref ref
Emotional only (2 or more) 0.30 (0.09, 0.51) 0.006 0.26 (0.04, 0.49) 0.021 0.17 (-0.13, 0.46) 0.264 0.07 (-0.23, 0.38) 0.635
Economic only (2 or more) 0.24 (-0.02, 0.49) 0.06 0.15 (-0.08, 0.39) 0.201 0.50 (0.11, 0.89) 0.013 0.42 (0.05, 0.80) 0.026
Emotional (2 or more) and economic (2 or more) 0.84 (0.60, 0.86) <0.001 0.59 (0.35, 0.83) <0.001 0.86 (0.34, 1.39) 0.001 0.54 (0.05, 1.03) 0.03
Emotional (2 or more) and physical/sexual (2 or more) 0.72 (0.59, 0.86) <0.001 0.47 (0.33, 0.83) <0.001 0.82 (0.49, 1.15) <0.001 0.51 (0.17, 0.86) 0.004
Economic (2 or more) and physical/sexual (2 or more) 0.77 (0.50, 1.04) <0.001 0.64 (0.37, 0.90) <0.001 0.76 (0.26, 1.27) 0.003 0.66 (0.16, 1.17) 0.01
Physical/Sexual only (2 or more) 0.42 (0.25, 0.59) <0.001 0.34 (0.17, 0.51) <0.001 0.49 (0.13, 0.85) 0.008 0.38 (0.00, 0.76) 0.051
All IPV experienced (2 or more) 0.93 (0.81, 1.05) <0.001 0.54 (0.40, 0.69) <0.001 1.28 (1.01, 1.56) <0.001 0.82 (0.53, 1.11) <0.001

*Negative binomial regression.

†Ordered logistic regression.

‡Adjusted for: age, education, poverty and childhood traumas.



For economic IPV, independent associations with worse health outcomes are stronger for self-reported health, particularly when coded as two or more experiences. In descriptive models, and adjusted models, when economic IPV is coded as two or more there are consistent associations with worse self-rated health, with no overlap of 95% CIs in descriptive models (Table S3 in the Online Supplementary Document), and in all adjusted models (Table 4, Model 8; Table 5, Model 10). There are less consistent findings when economic IPV is treated as one or more experiences. Similarly, there are more mixed findings for economic IPV and its association with depressive symptoms, where the models show inconsistent findings.

Across all models women who experience physical and/or sexual IPV, when combined with emotional or economic IPV consistently strong associations with worse health outcomes, and the highest scores for depression and self-rated health. For instance in Table S3 in the Online Supplementary Document, all combinations of combined IPV show the highest mean scores for symptoms. Similarly, in all the adjusted regression models, combined experiences of IPV are show the largest coefficients for depressive symptoms and self-rated health, and are in all models these associations are highly significant (Table 4 and Table 5).

DISCUSSION

In three population-based samples from the Asia-Pacific region (Sri Lanka, China, and PNG), lifetime experiences of emotional IPV and economic IPV were common for women. Emotional IPV was the most prevalent form of lifetime IPV assessed ranging from 29.5% in Sri Lanka, 37.7% in China, to 69.0% in PNG. Lifetime prevalence of economic IPV was also high, ranging from 24.7% in China, 27.0% in Sri Lanka, to 55.4% in PNG. The comparative prevalence of emotional IPV and economic IPV tracked closely with the prevalence of IPV by country with participants in PNG reporting the highest prevalence of physical IPV and sexual IPV, as well as emotional IPV, and economic IPV.

While emotional IPV and economic IPV were common across all three countries, there were differences in the most common items reported for each type of IPV by country. For emotional IPV, “scaring and intimidating” was the most common item in Sri Lanka and China, while in PNG it was “insulted”. There was little consistency in terms of ‘uniqueness’ of items by country. Only one item, ‘hurt others,’ had low uniqueness across the three countries, with <3% of women reporting emotional IPV reporting only that item. There was also variation in most prevalent form of economic IPV by country. In Sri Lanka and China ‘prohibit from working’ was the most common, while in PNG it was ‘taken earnings’. There was no item from the economic IPV scale, which had consistently low uniqueness.

The variation in the most common items of emotional and economic IPV and limited set of items with consistently low levels of uniqueness across the three countries has two implications. First, the variation in reporting across different countries suggests that while both emotional IPV and economic IPV are commonly experienced, the forms they take vary by context, and this is likely linked to different norms around relationships, women’s mobility, the expectations around women’s work, and male provision in relationships [9], as well as the nature and patterning of poverty. For example formal work in PNG was relatively uncommon (21% in this study), compared to the Sri Lanka and China where women’s employment is more common [21,22]. Similarly, men refusing to give women money when there was enough is also highly contingent on how the distribution of economic resources are understood in relationships.

Second, the current set of commonly used emotional IPV and economic IPV indicators cannot be easily reduced, as there were very items that across all countries had a low level of uniqueness. The only item that could potentially be removed to shorten questionnaires for emotional IPV was “hurt others” where <3% of women only reported that form of emotional IPV. If removed, in these three populations, the prevalence of emotional IPV would be about 3% less. As such, this suggests the current set of items do provide a unique set of items that do not repeat one another.

Our analysis clearly demonstrates that emotional IPV and economic IPV have negative health impacts on women, and that this impact is independent of the impact of co-occurring physical and/or sexual IPV. This was demonstrated across a range of different modelling and coding strategies, with consistent patterning of findings. Importantly, given the large data set we were able to parse out the multiple different combinations of IPV experienced by women, thus limiting the potential for hidden cofounders of IPV experience.

There was also greater consistency for the health impacts of emotional IPV and economic IPV (when excluding physical and/or sexual IPV), when these constructs were coded as “never or once” vs “two or more” experiences, rather than a “never/ever” binary. This suggests that coding emotional and economic IPV as none or once vs two or more, may be the appropriate way to capture a level of IPV where negative health impacts are seen.

The analysis suggests that while capturing emotional IPV and economic IPV in terms of frequency of reporting (never, once, few, many), may be useful in looking at severity, in reality emotional IPV and economic IPV functioned best as binary measures. When examining frequency and health impacts, there were inconsistencies in reporting, with some women reporting worse health outcomes for ‘few’ rather than ‘many’ experiences. The majority of women reporting any emotional IPV or economic IPV experienced this multiple times, rather than just once, and it may be that the response of never, once, few, or many, categories do not resonate with women’s experiences of these forms of violence, which tend not to be discrete experiences. As such, developing a range of responses that resonate more closely with women’s experiences may be an important future step.

Where women experienced multiple forms of IPV, ie, emotional and economic combined with physical and/or sexual IPV, they reported the worst health outcomes, and this is likely an indication of the severity of IPV experienced. This is the same finding as Gibbs et al [11] found in a non-representative sample in South African young women. Currently there is no agreed upon approach as to how to assess the severity of IPV, with some approaches suggesting two or more experiences of physical/sexual IPV [23], while others suggesting a more complex coding based on specific acts women experience [24]. This coding of women who experience physical and/or sexual IPV as well as emotional or economic IPV, may be a simple way of modelling severity, which is robust in population data.

Emotional IPV was consistently associated with depressive symptoms, but not self-rated generalised health, while economic IPV was more consistently associated with worse self-rated generalised health. This patterning was seen in both the individual items of each scale, and in the combined violence measures where different experiences were parsed out. It is unclear why these specific associations were seen, although the few other studies looking at both forms of IPV, have also demonstrated differences in health impacts [25]. This is suggestive that emotional IPV and economic IPV may operate somewhat differently and further research is required to understand this.

This study has a number of limitations. The measures of emotional IPV and economic IPV were for lifetime occurrence, while depressive symptoms were past week, and self-reported health the current period. As such, reports of IPV may have been from a long time prior to the survey. Despite this lack of congruence of reporting periods the pronounced impact on women’s health was still identified, which may be because many women experience IPV repeatedly, rather than just once-off. We only assessed depressive symptoms, rather than providing a clinical diagnosis and generalized health was a single item, and as such, the study needs repeating with more robust health outcomes. We did not create entirely “clean” referent categories for all analyses due to sample size issues, so in some cases a small proportion may have reported either emotional IPV or economic IPV, rather than truly “none”. The data were population representative samples and as such are generalizable to the wider population they came from, but the population was not always the whole country.

CONCLUSIONS

In conclusion, emotional IPV and economic IPV are more common, or as common as physical IPV and sexual IPV in three countries in Asia-Pacific. This analysis has highlighted that the current set of emotional and economic items used by the WHO captures a range of unique instances of violence that women face from their partner and that forms of emotional and economic IPV are patterned across different contexts. However the item list is unlikely to be exhaustive and future research may want to explore if important additional items need to be added for particular contexts. In addition, it has demonstrated that simple binary coding of these scales provides a robust way of providing a measure of health impact. The simplicity of this approach enables replication and standardization of measurement of these key constructs across multiple settings, enabling comparison.

Additional material
Acknowledgments

The UN Multi-country Study on Men and Violence, was coordinated and funded by Partners for Prevention, a UNDP, UNFPA, UN Women and UNV regional joint programme for gender-based violence prevention in Asia and the Pacific. The data for the individual countries is owned by UNFPA in China, UNDP in Bougainville, Papua New Guinea, CARE International in Sri Lanka. Data access is managed by the Sexual Violence Research Initiative (www.svri.org). 
The project was a collaborative effort and only made possible by the commitment, dedication, and hard work of all of the organizations and individuals involved, both internationally and in each of the study countries. First and foremost, we would like to acknowledge and give gratitude to the women who gave their time to participate in our study and generously shared their life experiences with us. We wish to express our profound thanks to our partner institutions and organizations in each of the study countries, as well as all of the interviewers and supervisors who worked tirelessly, and often under difficult circumstances, to collect the data for this study. We would like to thank the studies’ technical advisors, members of the steering committee, and members of the national working groups. We would like to thank all the members of the Partner for Prevention Technical Advisory Group and Regional Steering Committee who guided the overall study. 
UN Multi-country Study on Men and Violence Study Team:
Core research team: Emma Fulu (P4P) (Study Coordinator), Rachel Jewkes (Medical Research Council, South Africa), Xian Warner (P4P), Stephanie Miedema (P4P), Tim Roselli (P4P), and James Lang (P4P).
Country study teams – China: Dr Wang Xiangxian (PI) (Tianjin University, China); Fang Gang (Beijing Forestry University); Li Hongtao (Chinese Women's College and Anti-Domestic Violence Network); Zeljka Mudrovcic, Wen Hua, Arie Hoekman, Elina Nikulainen, Bernard Coquelin, and Mariam Khan (UNFPA China); Sri Lanka: Neloufer de Mel (PI) (University of Colombo); Pradeep Peiris (Social Scientists' Association); Shyamala Gomez (independent consultant); Social Indicator Team; and Kamani Jinadasa (CARE Sri Lanka); Papua New Guinea (Bougainville): Rachel Jewkes (PI), Yandisa Sikweyiya, and Nwabisa Shai (Medical Research Council, South Africa); Francesca Drapuluvik-Tinabar (National Statistics Office, PNG); Peterson Magoola and Anthony Agyenta (UNDP PNG); Thomas Shanahan and Tracy Vienings (UNDP Regional Pacific Centre). Steering committee: Rachel Jewkes (MRC, South Africa), Claudia Garcia-Moreno (WHO), Ruchira Tabassum Naved (ICDDR,B), Kamani Jinadasa (CARE Sri Lanka), Tracy Vienings (UNDP Regional Pacific Centre) and Wenny Kusuma (UN Women Cambodia). Technical advisory group: Rachel Jewkes (MRC, South Africa), Raewyn Connell (University of Sydney, Australia), Gary Barker (Instituto Promundo, USA & Brazil), Alan Greig (Independent consultant, USA), Rahul Roy (AAKAR, India), Ravi Verma (ICRW), Kalyani Menon Sen (Independent consultant). PDA programmer: Scott Johnson (University of Kentucky).

Disclaimer: The authors alone are responsible for the views expressed in this article, and they do not necessarily represent the views, decisions, or policies of the World Health Organization.

Notes

[1] Funding: This document is an output from the What Works to Prevent Violence: a Global Programme that is funded by the UK Aid from the UK Department for International Development (DFID) for the benefit of developing countries, managed by the South African Medical Research Council. The primary research was funded by Partners for Prevention, a UNDP, UNFPA, UN Women and UNV regional joint programme for gender-based violence prevention in Asia and the Pacific. Funding was provided by UNFPA, UN Women, UNDP, CARE, the Government of Australia, the Government of Sweden, the Government of Norway, and South African Medical Research Council. No funding bodies had any role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

[2] Authorship contributions: AG, KD & RJ conceptualized the study, and reviewed the analysis. AG conducted the analysis and wrote the first draft of the paper. KD and RJ supervised the analysis, contributed to interpretation and provided substantive inputs into the paper in draft form. All authors approved the final manuscript.

[3] Competing interests: The authors completed the ICMJE Unified Competing Interest form (available upon request from the corresponding author), and declare no conflicts of interest.

REFERENCES

[1] M Ellsberg, HAFM Jansen, L Heise, CH Watts, C Garcia-Moreno, and WMSW Hlth. Intimate partner violence and women’s physical and mental health in the WHO multi-country study on women’s health and domestic violence: an observational study. Lancet. 2008;371:1165-72. DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60522-X. [PMID:18395577]

[2] C García-Moreno and A Amin. The sustainable development goals, violence and women’s and children’s health. Bull World Health Organ. 2016;94:396 DOI: 10.2471/BLT.16.172205. [PMID:27147771]

[3] C Garcia-Moreno, HAFM Jansen, M Ellsberg, L Heise, CH Watts, and WM-CS Wo. Prevalence of intimate partner violence: findings from the WHO multi-country study on women’s health and domestic violence. Lancet. 2006;368:1260-9. DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)69523-8. [PMID:17027732]

[4] WHO. WHO multi-country study on women's health and domestic violence against women. Geneva: WHO, 2005.

[5] R Jewkes. Emotional abuse: a neglected dimension of partner violence. Lancet. 2010;376:851-2. DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)61079-3. [PMID:20822808]

[6] D Durevall and A Lindskog. Intimate partner violence and HIV in ten sub-Saharan African countries: what do the Demographic and Health Surveys tell us? Lancet Glob Health. 2015;3:DOI: 10.1016/S2214-109X(14)70343-2. [PMID:25539967]

[7] J Kutin, R Russell, and M Reid. Economic abuse between intimate partners in Australia: prevalence, health status, disability and financial stress. Aust N Z J Public Health. 2017;41:269-74. DOI: 10.1111/1753-6405.12651. [PMID:28245514]

[8] MM Haj-Yahia. Implications of wife abuse and battering for self-esteem, depression, and anxiety as revealed by the second Palestinian national survey on violence against women. J Fam Issues. 2000;21:435-63. DOI: 10.1177/019251300021004002

[9] Stern E, Gibbs A, Willan S, Dunkle K, Jewkes R. ‘When you talk to someone in a bad way or always put her under pressure, it is actually worse than beating her’: Conceptions and experiences of emotional intimate partner violence in Rwanda and South Africa. Under review.

[10] AB Ludermir, G Lewis, SA Valongueiro, TVB de Araújo, and R Araya. Violence against women by their intimate partner during pregnancy and postnatal depression: a prospective cohort study. Lancet. 2010;376:903-10. DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)60887-2. [PMID:20822809]

[11] A Gibbs, K Dunkle, and R Jewkes. Emotional and economic intimate partner violence as key drivers of depression and suicidal ideation: a cross-sectional study among young women in informal settlements in South Africa. Plos One. 2018;13:e0194885. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0194885. [PMID:29659595]

[12] H Stöckl and B Penhale. Intimate partner violence and its association with physical and mental health symptoms among older women in Germany. J Interpers Violence. 2015;30:3089-111. DOI: 10.1177/0886260514554427. [PMID:25392386]

[13] J McLaughlin, R O’carroll, and R O’connor. Intimate partner abuse and suicidality: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. 2012;32:677-89. DOI: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.08.002. [PMID:23017498]

[14] RT Naved and N Akhtar. Spousal violence against women and suicidal ideation in Bangladesh. Womens Health Issues. 2008;18:442-52. DOI: 10.1016/j.whi.2008.07.003. [PMID:19041596]

[15] I Seff and L Stark. A sex-disaggregated analysis of how emotional violence relates to suicide ideation in low-and middle-income countries. Child Abuse Negl. 2019;93:222-7. DOI: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2019.05.008. [PMID:31125852]

[16] Fulu E, Miedema S, Jewkes R, Roselli T, Lang J. Why Do Some Men Use Violence Against Women and How Can We Prevent It? Quantitative Findings from the United Nations Multi-country Study on Men and Violence in Asia and the Pacific. Bangkok: UNDP, UNFPA, UN Women and UNV, 2013.

[17] R Jewkes, E Fulu, RT Naved, E Chirwa, K Dunkle, and R Haardörfer. Women’s and men’s reports of past-year prevalence of intimate partner violence and rape and women’s risk factors for intimate partner violence: A multicountry cross-sectional study in Asia and the Pacific. PLoS Med. 2017;14:e1002381. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002381. [PMID:28873087]

[18] EM Andresen, JA Malmgren, WB Carter, and DL Patrick. Screening for depression in well older adults: Evaluation of a short form of the CES-D. Am J Prev Med. 1994;10:77-84. DOI: 10.1016/S0749-3797(18)30622-6. [PMID:8037935]

[19] T Björgvinsson, SJ Kertz, JS Bigda-Peyton, KL McCoy, and IM Aderka. Psychometric properties of the CES-D-10 in a psychiatric sample. Assessment. 2013;20:429-36. DOI: 10.1177/1073191113481998. [PMID:23513010]

[20] Paben S, editor. Comparison of Variance Estimation Methods for the National Compensation Survey. Proceedings of the Section on Survey Research Methods, American Statistical Association; 1999.

[21] P Hancock. Violence, women, work and empowerment: Narratives from factory women in Sri Lanka’s Export Processing Zones. Gend Technol Dev. 2006;10:211-28. DOI: 10.1177/097185240601000203

[22] Ngai P. Made in China: Women factory workers in a global workplace. Duke University Press; 2005.

[23] RK Jewkes, K Dunkle, M Nduna, and N Shai. Intimate partner violence, relationship power inequity, and incidence of HIV infection in young women in South Africa: a cohort study. Lancet. 2010;376:41-8. DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)60548-X. [PMID:20557928]

[24] Heise L, Hossain M. Measuring intimate partner violence. London: LSHTM, 2017.

[25] A Gibbs, K Dunkle, S Willan, N Jama-Shai, L Washington, and R Jewkes. Are women’s experiences of emotional and economic intimate partner violence associated with HIV-risk behaviour? A cross-sectional analysis of young women in informal settlements in South Africa. AIDS Care. 2019;31:667-74. DOI: 10.1080/09540121.2018.1533230. [PMID:30409025]



Journal of Global Health (ISSN 2047-2986), Edinburgh University Global Health Society
Designed by